<h1>ORIGIN OF SPECIES.</h1>
<p><SPAN name="link2H_INTR" id="link2H_INTR"></SPAN></p>
<h2> INTRODUCTION. </h2>
<p>When on board H.M.S. Beagle, as naturalist, I was much struck with certain
facts in the distribution of the organic beings inhabiting South America,
and in the geological relations of the present to the past inhabitants of
that continent. These facts, as will be seen in the latter chapters of
this volume, seemed to throw some light on the origin of species—that
mystery of mysteries, as it has been called by one of our greatest
philosophers. On my return home, it occurred to me, in 1837, that
something might perhaps be made out on this question by patiently
accumulating and reflecting on all sorts of facts which could possibly
have any bearing on it. After five years' work I allowed myself to
speculate on the subject, and drew up some short notes; these I enlarged
in 1844 into a sketch of the conclusions, which then seemed to me
probable: from that period to the present day I have steadily pursued the
same object. I hope that I may be excused for entering on these personal
details, as I give them to show that I have not been hasty in coming to a
decision.</p>
<p>My work is now (1859) nearly finished; but as it will take me many more
years to complete it, and as my health is far from strong, I have been
urged to publish this abstract. I have more especially been induced to do
this, as Mr. Wallace, who is now studying the natural history of the Malay
Archipelago, has arrived at almost exactly the same general conclusions
that I have on the origin of species. In 1858 he sent me a memoir on this
subject, with a request that I would forward it to Sir Charles Lyell, who
sent it to the Linnean Society, and it is published in the third volume of
the Journal of that Society. Sir C. Lyell and Dr. Hooker, who both knew of
my work—the latter having read my sketch of 1844—honoured me
by thinking it advisable to publish, with Mr. Wallace's excellent memoir,
some brief extracts from my manuscripts.</p>
<p>This abstract, which I now publish, must necessarily be imperfect. I
cannot here give references and authorities for my several statements; and
I must trust to the reader reposing some confidence in my accuracy. No
doubt errors may have crept in, though I hope I have always been cautious
in trusting to good authorities alone. I can here give only the general
conclusions at which I have arrived, with a few facts in illustration, but
which, I hope, in most cases will suffice. No one can feel more sensible
than I do of the necessity of hereafter publishing in detail all the
facts, with references, on which my conclusions have been grounded; and I
hope in a future work to do this. For I am well aware that scarcely a
single point is discussed in this volume on which facts cannot be adduced,
often apparently leading to conclusions directly opposite to those at
which I have arrived. A fair result can be obtained only by fully stating
and balancing the facts and arguments on both sides of each question; and
this is here impossible.</p>
<p>I much regret that want of space prevents my having the satisfaction of
acknowledging the generous assistance which I have received from very many
naturalists, some of them personally unknown to me. I cannot, however, let
this opportunity pass without expressing my deep obligations to Dr.
Hooker, who, for the last fifteen years, has aided me in every possible
way by his large stores of knowledge and his excellent judgment.</p>
<p>In considering the origin of species, it is quite conceivable that a
naturalist, reflecting on the mutual affinities of organic beings, on
their embryological relations, their geographical distribution, geological
succession, and other such facts, might come to the conclusion that
species had not been independently created, but had descended, like
varieties, from other species. Nevertheless, such a conclusion, even if
well founded, would be unsatisfactory, until it could be shown how the
innumerable species, inhabiting this world have been modified, so as to
acquire that perfection of structure and coadaptation which justly excites
our admiration. Naturalists continually refer to external conditions, such
as climate, food, etc., as the only possible cause of variation. In one
limited sense, as we shall hereafter see, this may be true; but it is
preposterous to attribute to mere external conditions, the structure, for
instance, of the woodpecker, with its feet, tail, beak, and tongue, so
admirably adapted to catch insects under the bark of trees. In the case of
the mistletoe, which draws its nourishment from certain trees, which has
seeds that must be transported by certain birds, and which has flowers
with separate sexes absolutely requiring the agency of certain insects to
bring pollen from one flower to the other, it is equally preposterous to
account for the structure of this parasite, with its relations to several
distinct organic beings, by the effects of external conditions, or of
habit, or of the volition of the plant itself.</p>
<p>It is, therefore, of the highest importance to gain a clear insight into
the means of modification and coadaptation. At the commencement of my
observations it seemed to me probable that a careful study of domesticated
animals and of cultivated plants would offer the best chance of making out
this obscure problem. Nor have I been disappointed; in this and in all
other perplexing cases I have invariably found that our knowledge,
imperfect though it be, of variation under domestication, afforded the
best and safest clue. I may venture to express my conviction of the high
value of such studies, although they have been very commonly neglected by
naturalists.</p>
<p>From these considerations, I shall devote the first chapter of this
abstract to variation under domestication. We shall thus see that a large
amount of hereditary modification is at least possible; and, what is
equally or more important, we shall see how great is the power of man in
accumulating by his selection successive slight variations. I will then
pass on to the variability of species in a state of nature; but I shall,
unfortunately, be compelled to treat this subject far too briefly, as it
can be treated properly only by giving long catalogues of facts. We shall,
however, be enabled to discuss what circumstances are most favourable to
variation. In the next chapter the struggle for existence among all
organic beings throughout the world, which inevitably follows from the
high geometrical ratio of their increase, will be considered. This is the
doctrine of Malthus, applied to the whole animal and vegetable kingdoms.
As many more individuals of each species are born than can possibly
survive; and as, consequently, there is a frequently recurring struggle
for existence, it follows that any being, if it vary however slightly in
any manner profitable to itself, under the complex and sometimes varying
conditions of life, will have a better chance of surviving, and thus be
NATURALLY SELECTED. From the strong principle of inheritance, any selected
variety will tend to propagate its new and modified form.</p>
<p>This fundamental subject of natural selection will be treated at some
length in the fourth chapter; and we shall then see how natural selection
almost inevitably causes much extinction of the less improved forms of
life, and leads to what I have called divergence of character. In the next
chapter I shall discuss the complex and little known laws of variation. In
the five succeeding chapters, the most apparent and gravest difficulties
in accepting the theory will be given: namely, first, the difficulties of
transitions, or how a simple being or a simple organ can be changed and
perfected into a highly developed being or into an elaborately constructed
organ; secondly the subject of instinct, or the mental powers of animals;
thirdly, hybridism, or the infertility of species and the fertility of
varieties when intercrossed; and fourthly, the imperfection of the
geological record. In the next chapter I shall consider the geological
succession of organic beings throughout time; in the twelfth and
thirteenth, their geographical distribution throughout space; in the
fourteenth, their classification or mutual affinities, both when mature
and in an embryonic condition. In the last chapter I shall give a brief
recapitulation of the whole work, and a few concluding remarks.</p>
<p>No one ought to feel surprise at much remaining as yet unexplained in
regard to the origin of species and varieties, if he make due allowance
for our profound ignorance in regard to the mutual relations of the many
beings which live around us. Who can explain why one species ranges widely
and is very numerous, and why another allied species has a narrow range
and is rare? Yet these relations are of the highest importance, for they
determine the present welfare and, as I believe, the future success and
modification of every inhabitant of this world. Still less do we know of
the mutual relations of the innumerable inhabitants of the world during
the many past geological epochs in its history. Although much remains
obscure, and will long remain obscure, I can entertain no doubt, after the
most deliberate study and dispassionate judgment of which I am capable,
that the view which most naturalists until recently entertained, and which
I formerly entertained—namely, that each species has been
independently created—is erroneous. I am fully convinced that
species are not immutable; but that those belonging to what are called the
same genera are lineal descendants of some other and generally extinct
species, in the same manner as the acknowledged varieties of any one
species are the descendants of that species. Furthermore, I am convinced
that natural selection has been the most important, but not the exclusive,
means of modification.</p>
<p><SPAN name="link2HCH0001" id="link2HCH0001"></SPAN></p>
<h2> CHAPTER I. VARIATION UNDER DOMESTICATION. </h2>
<p>Causes of Variability—Effects of Habit and the use and disuse of<br/>
Parts—Correlated Variation—Inheritance—Character of Domestic<br/>
Varieties—Difficulty of distinguishing between Varieties and<br/>
Species—Origin of Domestic Varieties from one or more Species—Domestic<br/>
Pigeons, their Differences and Origin—Principles of Selection,<br/>
anciently followed, their Effects—Methodical and Unconscious<br/>
Selection—Unknown Origin of our Domestic Productions—Circumstances<br/>
favourable to Man's power of Selection.<br/></p>
<p>CAUSES OF VARIABILITY.</p>
<p>When we compare the individuals of the same variety or sub-variety of our
older cultivated plants and animals, one of the first points which strikes
us is, that they generally differ more from each other than do the
individuals of any one species or variety in a state of nature. And if we
reflect on the vast diversity of the plants and animals which have been
cultivated, and which have varied during all ages under the most different
climates and treatment, we are driven to conclude that this great
variability is due to our domestic productions having been raised under
conditions of life not so uniform as, and somewhat different from, those
to which the parent species had been exposed under nature. There is, also,
some probability in the view propounded by Andrew Knight, that this
variability may be partly connected with excess of food. It seems clear
that organic beings must be exposed during several generations to new
conditions to cause any great amount of variation; and that, when the
organisation has once begun to vary, it generally continues varying for
many generations. No case is on record of a variable organism ceasing to
vary under cultivation. Our oldest cultivated plants, such as wheat, still
yield new varieties: our oldest domesticated animals are still capable of
rapid improvement or modification.</p>
<p>As far as I am able to judge, after long attending to the subject, the
conditions of life appear to act in two ways—directly on the whole
organisation or on certain parts alone and in directly by affecting the
reproductive system. With respect to the direct action, we must bear in
mind that in every case, as Professor Weismann has lately insisted, and as
I have incidently shown in my work on "Variation under Domestication,"
there are two factors: namely, the nature of the organism and the nature
of the conditions. The former seems to be much the more important; for
nearly similar variations sometimes arise under, as far as we can judge,
dissimilar conditions; and, on the other hand, dissimilar variations arise
under conditions which appear to be nearly uniform. The effects on the
offspring are either definite or in definite. They may be considered as
definite when all or nearly all the offspring of individuals exposed to
certain conditions during several generations are modified in the same
manner. It is extremely difficult to come to any conclusion in regard to
the extent of the changes which have been thus definitely induced. There
can, however, be little doubt about many slight changes, such as size from
the amount of food, colour from the nature of the food, thickness of the
skin and hair from climate, etc. Each of the endless variations which we
see in the plumage of our fowls must have had some efficient cause; and if
the same cause were to act uniformly during a long series of generations
on many individuals, all probably would be modified in the same manner.
Such facts as the complex and extraordinary out growths which variably
follow from the insertion of a minute drop of poison by a gall-producing
insect, shows us what singular modifications might result in the case of
plants from a chemical change in the nature of the sap.</p>
<p>In definite variability is a much more common result of changed conditions
than definite variability, and has probably played a more important part
in the formation of our domestic races. We see in definite variability in
the endless slight peculiarities which distinguish the individuals of the
same species, and which cannot be accounted for by inheritance from either
parent or from some more remote ancestor. Even strongly-marked differences
occasionally appear in the young of the same litter, and in seedlings from
the same seed-capsule. At long intervals of time, out of millions of
individuals reared in the same country and fed on nearly the same food,
deviations of structure so strongly pronounced as to deserve to be called
monstrosities arise; but monstrosities cannot be separated by any distinct
line from slighter variations. All such changes of structure, whether
extremely slight or strongly marked, which appear among many individuals
living together, may be considered as the in definite effects of the
conditions of life on each individual organism, in nearly the same manner
as the chill effects different men in an in definite manner, according to
their state of body or constitution, causing coughs or colds, rheumatism,
or inflammation of various organs.</p>
<p>With respect to what I have called the in direct action of changed
conditions, namely, through the reproductive system of being affected, we
may infer that variability is thus induced, partly from the fact of this
system being extremely sensitive to any change in the conditions, and
partly from the similarity, as Kolreuter and others have remarked, between
the variability which follows from the crossing of distinct species, and
that which may be observed with plants and animals when reared under new
or unnatural conditions. Many facts clearly show how eminently susceptible
the reproductive system is to very slight changes in the surrounding
conditions. Nothing is more easy than to tame an animal, and few things
more difficult than to get it to breed freely under confinement, even when
the male and female unite. How many animals there are which will not
breed, though kept in an almost free state in their native country! This
is generally, but erroneously attributed to vitiated instincts. Many
cultivated plants display the utmost vigour, and yet rarely or never seed!
In some few cases it has been discovered that a very trifling change, such
as a little more or less water at some particular period of growth, will
determine whether or not a plant will produce seeds. I cannot here give
the details which I have collected and elsewhere published on this curious
subject; but to show how singular the laws are which determine the
reproduction of animals under confinement, I may mention that carnivorous
animals, even from the tropics, breed in this country pretty freely under
confinement, with the exception of the plantigrades or bear family, which
seldom produce young; whereas, carnivorous birds, with the rarest
exception, hardly ever lay fertile eggs. Many exotic plants have pollen
utterly worthless, in the same condition as in the most sterile hybrids.
When, on the one hand, we see domesticated animals and plants, though
often weak and sickly, breeding freely under confinement; and when, on the
other hand, we see individuals, though taken young from a state of nature
perfectly tamed, long-lived, and healthy (of which I could give numerous
instances), yet having their reproductive system so seriously affected by
unperceived causes as to fail to act, we need not be surprised at this
system, when it does act under confinement, acting irregularly, and
producing offspring somewhat unlike their parents. I may add that as some
organisms breed freely under the most unnatural conditions—for
instance, rabbits and ferrets kept in hutches—showing that their
reproductive organs are not easily affected; so will some animals and
plants withstand domestication or cultivation, and vary very slightly—perhaps
hardly more than in a state of nature.</p>
<p>Some naturalists have maintained that all variations are connected with
the act of sexual reproduction; but this is certainly an error; for I have
given in another work a long list of "sporting plants;" as they are called
by gardeners; that is, of plants which have suddenly produced a single bud
with a new and sometimes widely different character from that of the other
buds on the same plant. These bud variations, as they may be named, can be
propagated by grafts, offsets, etc., and sometimes by seed. They occur
rarely under nature, but are far from rare under culture. As a single bud
out of many thousands produced year after year on the same tree under
uniform conditions, has been known suddenly to assume a new character; and
as buds on distinct trees, growing under different conditions, have
sometimes yielded nearly the same variety—for instance, buds on
peach-trees producing nectarines, and buds on common roses producing
moss-roses—we clearly see that the nature of the conditions is of
subordinate importance in comparison with the nature of the organism in
determining each particular form of variation; perhaps of not more
importance than the nature of the spark, by which a mass of combustible
matter is ignited, has in determining the nature of the flames.</p>
<p>EFFECTS OF HABIT AND OF THE USE OR DISUSE OF PARTS; CORRELATED VARIATION;
INHERITANCE.</p>
<p>Changed habits produce an inherited effect as in the period of the
flowering of plants when transported from one climate to another. With
animals the increased use or disuse of parts has had a more marked
influence; thus I find in the domestic duck that the bones of the wing
weigh less and the bones of the leg more, in proportion to the whole
skeleton, than do the same bones in the wild duck; and this change may be
safely attributed to the domestic duck flying much less, and walking more,
than its wild parents. The great and inherited development of the udders
in cows and goats in countries where they are habitually milked, in
comparison with these organs in other countries, is probably another
instance of the effects of use. Not one of our domestic animals can be
named which has not in some country drooping ears; and the view which has
been suggested that the drooping is due to disuse of the muscles of the
ear, from the animals being seldom much alarmed, seems probable.</p>
<p>Many laws regulate variation, some few of which can be dimly seen, and
will hereafter be briefly discussed. I will here only allude to what may
be called correlated variation. Important changes in the embryo or larva
will probably entail changes in the mature animal. In monstrosities, the
correlations between quite distinct parts are very curious; and many
instances are given in Isidore Geoffroy St. Hilaire's great work on this
subject. Breeders believe that long limbs are almost always accompanied by
an elongated head. Some instances of correlation are quite whimsical; thus
cats which are entirely white and have blue eyes are generally deaf; but
it has been lately stated by Mr. Tait that this is confined to the males.
Colour and constitutional peculiarities go together, of which many
remarkable cases could be given among animals and plants. From facts
collected by Heusinger, it appears that white sheep and pigs are injured
by certain plants, while dark-coloured individuals escape: Professor Wyman
has recently communicated to me a good illustration of this fact; on
asking some farmers in Virginia how it was that all their pigs were black,
they informed him that the pigs ate the paint-root (Lachnanthes), which
coloured their bones pink, and which caused the hoofs of all but the black
varieties to drop off; and one of the "crackers" (i.e. Virginia squatters)
added, "we select the black members of a litter for raising, as they alone
have a good chance of living." Hairless dogs have imperfect teeth;
long-haired and coarse-haired animals are apt to have, as is asserted,
long or many horns; pigeons with feathered feet have skin between their
outer toes; pigeons with short beaks have small feet, and those with long
beaks large feet. Hence if man goes on selecting, and thus augmenting, any
peculiarity, he will almost certainly modify unintentionally other parts
of the structure, owing to the mysterious laws of correlation.</p>
<p>The results of the various, unknown, or but dimly understood laws of
variation are infinitely complex and diversified. It is well worth while
carefully to study the several treatises on some of our old cultivated
plants, as on the hyacinth, potato, even the dahlia, etc.; and it is
really surprising to note the endless points of structure and constitution
in which the varieties and sub-varieties differ slightly from each other.
The whole organisation seems to have become plastic, and departs in a
slight degree from that of the parental type.</p>
<p>Any variation which is not inherited is unimportant for us. But the number
and diversity of inheritable deviations of structure, both those of slight
and those of considerable physiological importance, are endless. Dr.
Prosper Lucas' treatise, in two large volumes, is the fullest and the best
on this subject. No breeder doubts how strong is the tendency to
inheritance; that like produces like is his fundamental belief: doubts
have been thrown on this principle only by theoretical writers. When any
deviation of structure often appears, and we see it in the father and
child, we cannot tell whether it may not be due to the same cause having
acted on both; but when among individuals, apparently exposed to the same
conditions, any very rare deviation, due to some extraordinary combination
of circumstances, appears in the parent—say, once among several
million individuals—and it reappears in the child, the mere doctrine
of chances almost compels us to attribute its reappearance to inheritance.
Every one must have heard of cases of albinism, prickly skin, hairy
bodies, etc., appearing in several members of the same family. If strange
and rare deviations of structure are truly inherited, less strange and
commoner deviations may be freely admitted to be inheritable. Perhaps the
correct way of viewing the whole subject would be, to look at the
inheritance of every character whatever as the rule, and non-inheritance
as the anomaly.</p>
<p>The laws governing inheritance are for the most part unknown; no one can
say why the same peculiarity in different individuals of the same species,
or in different species, is sometimes inherited and sometimes not so; why
the child often reverts in certain characteristics to its grandfather or
grandmother or more remote ancestor; why a peculiarity is often
transmitted from one sex to both sexes, or to one sex alone, more commonly
but not exclusively to the like sex. It is a fact of some importance to
us, that peculiarities appearing in the males of our domestic breeds are
often transmitted, either exclusively or in a much greater degree, to the
males alone. A much more important rule, which I think may be trusted, is
that, at whatever period of life a peculiarity first appears, it tends to
reappear in the offspring at a corresponding age, though sometimes
earlier. In many cases this could not be otherwise; thus the inherited
peculiarities in the horns of cattle could appear only in the offspring
when nearly mature; peculiarities in the silk-worm are known to appear at
the corresponding caterpillar or cocoon stage. But hereditary diseases and
some other facts make me believe that the rule has a wider extension, and
that, when there is no apparent reason why a peculiarity should appear at
any particular age, yet that it does tend to appear in the offspring at
the same period at which it first appeared in the parent. I believe this
rule to be of the highest importance in explaining the laws of embryology.
These remarks are of course confined to the first APPEARANCE of the
peculiarity, and not to the primary cause which may have acted on the
ovules or on the male element; in nearly the same manner as the increased
length of the horns in the offspring from a short-horned cow by a
long-horned bull, though appearing late in life, is clearly due to the
male element.</p>
<p>Having alluded to the subject of reversion, I may here refer to a
statement often made by naturalists—namely, that our domestic
varieties, when run wild, gradually but invariably revert in character to
their aboriginal stocks. Hence it has been argued that no deductions can
be drawn from domestic races to species in a state of nature. I have in
vain endeavoured to discover on what decisive facts the above statement
has so often and so boldly been made. There would be great difficulty in
proving its truth: we may safely conclude that very many of the most
strongly marked domestic varieties could not possibly live in a wild
state. In many cases we do not know what the aboriginal stock was, and so
could not tell whether or not nearly perfect reversion had ensued. It
would be necessary, in order to prevent the effects of intercrossing, that
only a single variety should be turned loose in its new home.
Nevertheless, as our varieties certainly do occasionally revert in some of
their characters to ancestral forms, it seems to me not improbable that if
we could succeed in naturalising, or were to cultivate, during many
generations, the several races, for instance, of the cabbage, in very poor
soil—in which case, however, some effect would have to be attributed
to the DEFINITE action of the poor soil—that they would, to a large
extent, or even wholly, revert to the wild aboriginal stock. Whether or
not the experiment would succeed is not of great importance for our line
of argument; for by the experiment itself the conditions of life are
changed. If it could be shown that our domestic varieties manifested a
strong tendency to reversion—that is, to lose their acquired
characters, while kept under the same conditions and while kept in a
considerable body, so that free intercrossing might check, by blending
together, any slight deviations in their structure, in such case, I grant
that we could deduce nothing from domestic varieties in regard to species.
But there is not a shadow of evidence in favour of this view: to assert
that we could not breed our cart and race-horses, long and short-horned
cattle, and poultry of various breeds, and esculent vegetables, for an
unlimited number of generations, would be opposed to all experience.</p>
<p>CHARACTER OF DOMESTIC VARIETIES; DIFFICULTY OF DISTINGUISHING BETWEEN
VARIETIES AND SPECIES; ORIGIN OF DOMESTIC VARIETIES FROM ONE OR MORE
SPECIES.</p>
<p>When we look to the hereditary varieties or races of our domestic animals
and plants, and compare them with closely allied species, we generally
perceive in each domestic race, as already remarked, less uniformity of
character than in true species. Domestic races often have a somewhat
monstrous character; by which I mean, that, although differing from each
other and from other species of the same genus, in several trifling
respects, they often differ in an extreme degree in some one part, both
when compared one with another, and more especially when compared with the
species under nature to which they are nearest allied. With these
exceptions (and with that of the perfect fertility of varieties when
crossed—a subject hereafter to be discussed), domestic races of the
same species differ from each other in the same manner as do the closely
allied species of the same genus in a state of nature, but the differences
in most cases are less in degree. This must be admitted as true, for the
domestic races of many animals and plants have been ranked by some
competent judges as the descendants of aboriginally distinct species, and
by other competent judges as mere varieties. If any well marked
distinction existed between a domestic race and a species, this source of
doubt would not so perpetually recur. It has often been stated that
domestic races do not differ from each other in characters of generic
value. It can be shown that this statement is not correct; but naturalists
differ much in determining what characters are of generic value; all such
valuations being at present empirical. When it is explained how genera
originate under nature, it will be seen that we have no right to expect
often to find a generic amount of difference in our domesticated races.</p>
<p>In attempting to estimate the amount of structural difference between
allied domestic races, we are soon involved in doubt, from not knowing
whether they are descended from one or several parent species. This point,
if it could be cleared up, would be interesting; if, for instance, it
could be shown that the greyhound, bloodhound, terrier, spaniel and
bull-dog, which we all know propagate their kind truly, were the offspring
of any single species, then such facts would have great weight in making
us doubt about the immutability of the many closely allied natural species—for
instance, of the many foxes—inhabiting the different quarters of the
world. I do not believe, as we shall presently see, that the whole amount
of difference between the several breeds of the dog has been produced
under domestication; I believe that a small part of the difference is due
to their being descended from distinct species. In the case of strongly
marked races of some other domesticated species, there is presumptive or
even strong evidence that all are descended from a single wild stock.</p>
<p>It has often been assumed that man has chosen for domestication animals
and plants having an extraordinary inherent tendency to vary, and likewise
to withstand diverse climates. I do not dispute that these capacities have
added largely to the value of most of our domesticated productions; but
how could a savage possibly know, when he first tamed an animal, whether
it would vary in succeeding generations, and whether it would endure other
climates? Has the little variability of the ass and goose, or the small
power of endurance of warmth by the reindeer, or of cold by the common
camel, prevented their domestication? I cannot doubt that if other animals
and plants, equal in number to our domesticated productions, and belonging
to equally diverse classes and countries, were taken from a state of
nature, and could be made to breed for an equal number of generations
under domestication, they would on an average vary as largely as the
parent species of our existing domesticated productions have varied.</p>
<p>In the case of most of our anciently domesticated animals and plants, it
is not possible to come to any definite conclusion, whether they are
descended from one or several wild species. The argument mainly relied on
by those who believe in the multiple origin of our domestic animals is,
that we find in the most ancient times, on the monuments of Egypt, and in
the lake-habitations of Switzerland, much diversity in the breeds; and
that some of these ancient breeds closely resemble, or are even identical
with, those still existing. But this only throws far backward the history
of civilisation, and shows that animals were domesticated at a much
earlier period than has hitherto been supposed. The lake-inhabitants of
Switzerland cultivated several kinds of wheat and barley, the pea, the
poppy for oil and flax; and they possessed several domesticated animals.
They also carried on commerce with other nations. All this clearly shows,
as Heer has remarked, that they had at this early age progressed
considerably in civilisation; and this again implies a long continued
previous period of less advanced civilisation, during which the
domesticated animals, kept by different tribes in different districts,
might have varied and given rise to distinct races. Since the discovery of
flint tools in the superficial formations of many parts of the world, all
geologists believe that barbarian men existed at an enormously remote
period; and we know that at the present day there is hardly a tribe so
barbarous as not to have domesticated at least the dog.</p>
<p>The origin of most of our domestic animals will probably forever remain
vague. But I may here state that, looking to the domestic dogs of the
whole world, I have, after a laborious collection of all known facts, come
to the conclusion that several wild species of Canidae have been tamed,
and that their blood, in some cases mingled together, flows in the veins
of our domestic breeds. In regard to sheep and goats I can form no decided
opinion. From facts communicated to me by Mr. Blyth, on the habits, voice,
constitution and structure of the humped Indian cattle, it is almost
certain that they are descended from a different aboriginal stock from our
European cattle; and some competent judges believe that these latter have
had two or three wild progenitors, whether or not these deserve to be
called species. This conclusion, as well as that of the specific
distinction between the humped and common cattle, may, indeed, be looked
upon as established by the admirable researches of Professor Rutimeyer.
With respect to horses, from reasons which I cannot here give, I am
doubtfully inclined to believe, in opposition to several authors, that all
the races belong to the same species. Having kept nearly all the English
breeds of the fowl alive, having bred and crossed them, and examined their
skeletons, it appears to me almost certain that all are the descendants of
the wild Indian fowl, Gallus bankiva; and this is the conclusion of Mr.
Blyth, and of others who have studied this bird in India. In regard to
ducks and rabbits, some breeds of which differ much from each other, the
evidence is clear that they are all descended from the common duck and
wild rabbit.</p>
<p>The doctrine of the origin of our several domestic races from several
aboriginal stocks, has been carried to an absurd extreme by some authors.
They believe that every race which breeds true, let the distinctive
characters be ever so slight, has had its wild prototype. At this rate
there must have existed at least a score of species of wild cattle, as
many sheep, and several goats, in Europe alone, and several even within
Great Britain. One author believes that there formerly existed eleven wild
species of sheep peculiar to Great Britain! When we bear in mind that
Britain has now not one peculiar mammal, and France but few distinct from
those of Germany, and so with Hungary, Spain, etc., but that each of these
kingdoms possesses several peculiar breeds of cattle, sheep, etc., we must
admit that many domestic breeds must have originated in Europe; for whence
otherwise could they have been derived? So it is in India. Even in the
case of the breeds of the domestic dog throughout the world, which I admit
are descended from several wild species, it cannot be doubted that there
has been an immense amount of inherited variation; for who will believe
that animals closely resembling the Italian greyhound, the bloodhound, the
bull-dog, pug-dog, or Blenheim spaniel, etc.—so unlike all wild
Canidae—ever existed in a state of nature? It has often been loosely
said that all our races of dogs have been produced by the crossing of a
few aboriginal species; but by crossing we can only get forms in some
degree intermediate between their parents; and if we account for our
several domestic races by this process, we must admit the former existence
of the most extreme forms, as the Italian greyhound, bloodhound, bull-dog,
etc., in the wild state. Moreover, the possibility of making distinct
races by crossing has been greatly exaggerated. Many cases are on record
showing that a race may be modified by occasional crosses if aided by the
careful selection of the individuals which present the desired character;
but to obtain a race intermediate between two quite distinct races would
be very difficult. Sir J. Sebright expressly experimented with this object
and failed. The offspring from the first cross between two pure breeds is
tolerably and sometimes (as I have found with pigeons) quite uniform in
character, and every thing seems simple enough; but when these mongrels
are crossed one with another for several generations, hardly two of them
are alike, and then the difficulty of the task becomes manifest.</p>
<div style="break-after:column;"></div><br />